Back

commented on Context Changes Everything by Alicia Juarrero

Alicia Juarrero: Context Changes Everything (2023, MIT Press, The MIT Press) 1 star

#JuarreroBook Chapter 6 Part 1 There's a lot in this chapter, and some of it I find hard to understand. So I'd like to split things up. We are now on context dependent constraints, the nature of which is to "take conditions away from independence"

The ch. outlines "three examples of the emergence of long-range correlations generated in virtue of context-dependent constraints. The first serves as a metaphor of phase transitions. The second illustrates inter-dependent dynamics among oscillators. The third is the textbook case of self- organizing, nonlinear, and far from equilibrium processes in the natural world. All three show how context-dependent constraints, operating against a backdrop established by context-independent constraints, weave global forms of order".

The examples are: 1. the phase transition of a random graph with sufficiently many links that it moves to connectedness

  1. synchronising pendulum clocks on a shelf

  2. convection patterns such as Bernard cells

What do …

@uh @UlrikeHahn@fediscience.org @dcm@social.sunet.se

Indeed this is a much longer chapter! I stopped at p. 73 for now.

I share your concern about the ontology here: Juarrero talks constantly about constraints 'doing' things, and even being a form of causality (though she doesn't say how). But her examples suggest that what she calls constraints are just ways of describing patterns that appear when certain entities interact with each other in specific organised ways. This impression is reinforced by the apparently circular treatment of context-dependent constraints on p.70: they are characterised by appeal to constrained interactions...

But then, rather than being something ontologically additional that does things, constraints are just ways of talking about features of such patterns, which are in their turn constituted by the familiar kinds of causal interactions between entities. So, nothing ontologically new, just, at most, new-ish alternative explanatory tools.

(This connects, I think, to the Deacon vs …

@dcm@bookwyrm.social @UlrikeHahn @NicoleCRust @dcm@social.sunet.se @awaisaftab @uh

Full disclosure: I'm not reading Juarrero’s book. But I'm really enjoying your discussions.

I have a simple view of DST as an idea that literally models how nature moves forward. The motion aspect is important to me because in my field we want to understand development — cognitive, social/emotional, behavioural. DST helps me make sense of development and how internal and external circumstances both contribute to and constrain experience...

@dcm@bookwyrm.social @UlrikeHahn @NicoleCRust @dcm@social.sunet.se @awaisaftab @uh

In most vertebrate species, the visual sensory system eventually turns the tables to dominate the auditory system. I believe that occurs in birds in the first few days of life with little individual difference. In humans, generally speaking, visual dominance takes a few years — with high context-specificity and considerable individual difference.

@dcm@bookwyrm.social @UlrikeHahn @NicoleCRust @dcm@social.sunet.se @awaisaftab @uh

Due credit to Gottlieb et al.(2006) who make mention of a "general lack of appreciation of the role of functional constraints or limitations in the realization of early patterns of perceptual organization."

Gottlieb et al. (2006). The Significance of Biology for Human Developmnt: A Developmental Psychobiological Systems View. In Lerner & Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 210–257). Wiley.