When editor Susan Ryeland is given the manuscript of Alan Conway’s latest novel, she has …
Review of 'Magpie Murders' on 'Goodreads'
4 stars
I like books with meta narratives. I also like puzzles, and this book entangles both in quite a clever way.
It also teases the reader by throwing in pieces of information that are obviously pertinent, and avoids explaining them for as long as possible, or flat out tells the reader that a conclusion turned out to be false, but again not explaining why for as long as possible.
If any of the above sounds tantalizing, this book is for you.
If on a Winter's Night a Traveler is a marvel of ingenuity, an experimental text …
Review of "If on a Winter's Night a Traveler" on 'Goodreads'
4 stars
I can't really decide what I feel about this book. It seems to be equal parts esoteric points about reading and writing books on one hand, and masterfully executed writing tricks on the other. I both enjoyed it immensely and thought it was hard to read. All in all, four stars, I guess? I don't know.
What William Gibson did for science fiction, China Mieville has done for fantasy, shattering old …
Review of 'Looking for Jake' on 'Goodreads'
5 stars
As always, China Miéville manages to be captivating. Especially The Tain manages to evoke a dirty, disturbing sense of horror and defeat, in a post apocalypse like none I've ever seen.
In this book, Elinor Ostrom examines the dismal "tragedy of the commons" argument that is so widely accepted and frequently used to advocate for private ownership as a mechanism to "protect" common pool resources (and other types of resources).
For this purpose, she presents real life case studies of both successful and failed attempts to utilize common pool resources by multiple actors, and showing how they do not fit the simple theoretical models of the "rational" actor with perfect information that leads inevitably to the tragedy of the commons. She then attempts to define a theoretical framework for models to analyze CPRs more accurately, taking into account the complexities of reality and a more nuanced characterization of human behaviour.
All in all it felt to me like Elinor Ostrom was awarded the Nobel prize in Economics because she reminded economists that humans aren't simply greedy, omniscient, homogeneous monsters who will …
In this book, Elinor Ostrom examines the dismal "tragedy of the commons" argument that is so widely accepted and frequently used to advocate for private ownership as a mechanism to "protect" common pool resources (and other types of resources).
For this purpose, she presents real life case studies of both successful and failed attempts to utilize common pool resources by multiple actors, and showing how they do not fit the simple theoretical models of the "rational" actor with perfect information that leads inevitably to the tragedy of the commons. She then attempts to define a theoretical framework for models to analyze CPRs more accurately, taking into account the complexities of reality and a more nuanced characterization of human behaviour.
All in all it felt to me like Elinor Ostrom was awarded the Nobel prize in Economics because she reminded economists that humans aren't simply greedy, omniscient, homogeneous monsters who will blindly maximize their own benefit at any cost to others or the environment, but rather varied beings with a spectrum of intentions, and with the capacity to think ahead, act strategically, and even sometimes cooperate to solve their common problems.
I read this book looking for analyses of different ways to organize in federations, and to govern without individual property. I didn't really get what I signed up for, but I got something equally interesting and very related: a systematic study that indicates that yes, even the social sciences agree that people can sometimes organize and accomplish remarkable things.
A provocative new collection of short stories by the New York Times best-selling and Hugo …
Review of 'Three moments of an explosion' on 'Goodreads'
4 stars
I finally found a China Miéville book that is not an automatic 5 for me.
That being said, I loved it.
This is a book with stories of varying lengths. Here, too, as in all of his books, China Miéville showcases how good he is at conjuring up interesting and rich premises for fantasy worlds. In this book, the worlds are all based on our own with a twist. I don't want to spoil it a lot, but just to give an idea, some of the twists that are widely advertised from this book are:
- A world where broken oil rigs come alive and start roaming the world. - A world where Earth built huge space elevators who form their own societies. - A world where Poker players around the world are visited by magical cards that are both wondrous and strict.
Some of the stories have more obvious …
I finally found a China Miéville book that is not an automatic 5 for me.
That being said, I loved it.
This is a book with stories of varying lengths. Here, too, as in all of his books, China Miéville showcases how good he is at conjuring up interesting and rich premises for fantasy worlds. In this book, the worlds are all based on our own with a twist. I don't want to spoil it a lot, but just to give an idea, some of the twists that are widely advertised from this book are:
- A world where broken oil rigs come alive and start roaming the world. - A world where Earth built huge space elevators who form their own societies. - A world where Poker players around the world are visited by magical cards that are both wondrous and strict.
Some of the stories have more obvious points, political, aesthetic or just conversational. Others are just weird (which is to say their points or ideas are more hidden and convoluted).
One thing all stories have in common, though, which gives this collection its character, is that all of them end abruptly. The book mostly lacks resolutions, and even when they exist, stories still end abruptly. This left me with a feeling of frustration, bewilderment and wonder, but thinking back, it feels like the concepts aren't made to sustain full books or really long winding plots: they're made to be briefly explored, as flights of fancy, and then abandoned at just the right moment before one flies to the next one.
The reason I ended up giving the book a four, was that I did feel unnecessarily confused at times, and I ended up not being full captivated. Since the stories were short, though, even when a story wasn't as captivating as I'd hoped, I quickly found myself being immersed in the next one.
All in all, this book is perhaps a little niche: don't read it if you expect to understand everything, or get satisfying conclusions: read it if you enjoy clever confusion, and jumping around in fantastical constructions.
Review of 'Everybody Lies: Big Data, New Data, and What the Internet Can Tell Us About Who We Really Are' on 'Goodreads'
2 stars
This book was fun, but it felt all too often that the conclusions it draws from the data it presented, didn't really follow from the data - or rather, it does to an extent, but with considerations.
This is a relevant passage from the book:
You might think that a terrorist attack that kills dozens or hundreds of people would automatically be followed by massive, widespread anxiety. Terrorism, by definition, is supposed to instill a sense of terror. I looked at Google searches reflecting anxiety. I tested how much these searches rose in a country the days, weeks, and months following every major European or American terrorist attack since 2004. So, on average, how much did anxiety-related searches rise? They didn't. At all.
The book relies heavily on Google search data and draws all sorts of conclusions from them, making the implicit assumption that Google searches follow people's feelings and …
This book was fun, but it felt all too often that the conclusions it draws from the data it presented, didn't really follow from the data - or rather, it does to an extent, but with considerations.
This is a relevant passage from the book:
You might think that a terrorist attack that kills dozens or hundreds of people would automatically be followed by massive, widespread anxiety. Terrorism, by definition, is supposed to instill a sense of terror. I looked at Google searches reflecting anxiety. I tested how much these searches rose in a country the days, weeks, and months following every major European or American terrorist attack since 2004. So, on average, how much did anxiety-related searches rise? They didn't. At all.
The book relies heavily on Google search data and draws all sorts of conclusions from them, making the implicit assumption that Google searches follow people's feelings and opinions. It also assumes that people are more honest with Google than with surveys, for various reasons (which I would agree with).
However, I would think it's obvious that people's Google searches (and many other such datasets used in the book), often can't follow what people are thinking or feeling very closely. The reason is that Google is a way to research something, or to remember something. People don't Google all of their feelings, thoughts and worries, even though the book posits that they Google more of them than we tend to think. To illustrate simply, towards the above quote, after a terrorist attack, people might be scared to go to crowded places, they might feel anxious, they might fear for their loved ones safety. I'm not sure any of these necessarily lead to Google searches. People will probably Google the event, they will read about it in the paper, they will adjust their schedules accordingly to perhaps go to the place the event happened to leave flowers, or avoid it altogether out of fear. They might make an extra appointment with their therapist, or call their parents or their friends. They might even Google something (like the way to the place of the event). But lack of Google searches related to anxiety does not mean a lack of anxiety.
A classic (albeit simplistic) example is the amounts of Google search results for "gay sex" compared to those for "straight sex". "gay sex" returns about 30% more results than "straight sex" as of the time of this writing. Does that mean people have a lot more gay sex than straight sex? Or that people think a lot more about gay sex than straight sex? No. Partly, this can be explained by the fact that straight people probably don't qualify their searches with "straight". They largely search for "sex". Or, well, they search for something more specific, as Googling for "sex" probably isn't something people do when they think about sex.
Similarly, there was another passage conjecturing about suicide, which mentioned the rates at which people Google searches related to suicide. The chapter also talked about how actual suicide rates are far lower than Googling for suicide, which means many people consider it without following up. To which I immediately think of many ways the group of people considering suicide and that of people Googling suicide can diverge: some people just Google suicide or ways to commit suicide out of curiosity. Some, who actually consider suicide, will Google it a couple of times, and then find information on it which they can keep - and thus not have to Google for it again. In general, when people Google, they often find a community or a website which can solve their questions on something that interests them, and they bookmark or subscribe to that website. Or they keep the information and don't need to ask Google about it again.
It's a bit like measuring how many times a certain theatre is visited, using a dataset with all the occasions where people ask someone on the street for directions.
One can claim that people mostly don't ask for directions anymore (probably because they can Google their way to the theatre). Thus, my comparison is unfair, as people Google their thoughts a lot more than they ask for directions.
This highlights my opinion on the matter: do we really know to which extent people Google their thoughts? Is it the same per topic? One thing is for sure: there's a lot of phenomena that explain why real life conditions cannot be construed from Google searches, and until these can be measured, quantified or explained away, any conclusions drawn from such data is inaccurate in unspecified ways, and shouldn't be trusted.
All in all, read this book because it presents fun data. Don't expect much from the conclusions.
some people, for example, simply Google "is my husband gay", which is clearly something Google cannot answer for that specific husband, but simply reveals what this person worries about.